Medical and Dental Consultantsí Association of Nigeria
Home - About us - Editorial board - Search - Ahead of print - Current issue - Archives - Submit article - Instructions - Subscribe - Advertise - Contacts - Login 
  Users Online: 1333   Home Print this page Email this page Small font sizeDefault font sizeIncrease font size
 

  Table of Contents 
ORIGINAL ARTICLE
Year : 2017  |  Volume : 20  |  Issue : 9  |  Page : 1150-1155

Environmental factors that determine visual skill development of under-fives in a developing country


1 Department of Paediatrics, College of Health Sciences, Bingham University/Bingham University Teaching Hospital, Jos, Nigeria
2 Department of Paediatrics, Ahmadu Bello University/Ahmadu Bello University Teaching Hospital, Zaria, Nigeria
3 Department of Ophthalmology, Ahmadu Bello University Teaching Hospital, Zaria, Nigeria

Date of Acceptance29-Apr-2017
Date of Web Publication26-Oct-2017

Correspondence Address:
A O Jimoh
Department of Paediatrics, College of Health Sciences, Bingham University/Bingham University Teaching Hospital, Jos - 930 214
Nigeria
Login to access the Email id

Source of Support: None, Conflict of Interest: None


DOI: 10.4103/njcp.njcp_438_16

Rights and Permissions
   Abstract 

Introduction: During the early years of life, children get most of their information by relying on their visual observation. Knowledge of visual skill development and environmental risk factors influencing it provides useful guide for early identification of children who may develop some form of visual impairment. Aim: The aim of this study is to describe the visual developmental pattern and determine the environmental risk factors associated with delay in the visual skill area of under-five children. Subjects and Methods: This was a cross-sectional study of 415 preschoolers aged 6–59 months. Visual function and visual comprehension were assessed using the Schedule of Growing Skills II tool (GL Assessment Ltd., London). Delay in the visual skill was defined as a developmental quotient in visual skill area below threshold point of 85%. Data were analyzed using descriptive statistics and logistic regression analysis with adjusted odds ratio (AOR) and 95% confidence interval (CI) (95% CI). Alpha value was set at P < 0.05. Results: Mean age of the children studied was 32.6 ± 15.9 months. The prevalence of delay in visual skill area was 17.1%. The odds of delay in visual skill were higher among children of first birth order (AOR 1.83; 95% CI 1.05–3.30), those who lived in large households (AOR 2.34; 95% CI 1.32–3.14), children whose mothers had secondary level education and below (AOR 2.21; 95% CI 1.31–3.83), and those whose fathers earned ≤$100 per month (AOR 1.75; 95% CI 1.01–3.03). Conclusion: Identification and management of environmental factors negatively affecting visual skill development will help improve on the visual skill area and invariably child development.

Keywords: Developing country, development, environmental factors, under-fives, visual skill


How to cite this article:
Jimoh A O, Anyiam J O, Abdulsalami H A, Orugun A J, Yakubu A M. Environmental factors that determine visual skill development of under-fives in a developing country. Niger J Clin Pract 2017;20:1150-5

How to cite this URL:
Jimoh A O, Anyiam J O, Abdulsalami H A, Orugun A J, Yakubu A M. Environmental factors that determine visual skill development of under-fives in a developing country. Niger J Clin Pract [serial online] 2017 [cited 2021 Sep 17];20:1150-5. Available from: https://www.njcponline.com/text.asp?2017/20/9/1150/217255


   Introduction Top


During the early years of life, children get more information rapidly by relying on their vision than any other sense.[1] The negative impact of poor vision goes far beyond the immediate impairment. Deficits in visual acuity, visual efficiency, and visual information processing can lead to difficulties in reading and writing, poor school performance, poor cognitive, social and behavioral development, and ultimately reduced the quality of life and reduced earning opportunities.[2],[3],[4],[5],[6] Early detection and identification of risk factors, on the other hand, reduce the burden of the disease, improve the general developmental outcome, and provide useful guide for detection of children at the risk of visual impairment. Regular vision screening in early childhood, for instance, has been found to reduce the risk of persistent amblyopia at 7 years of age by more than 50%.[7] This study aimed to expand the knowledge about visual skill development in under-fives by describing the developmental pattern and determining the environmental factors associated with delay in the skill area.


   Subjects and Methods Top


Study area and participants

The study was a cross-sectional study, conducted in institutions where apparently well under-fives are seen. These are selected preschool units (crèches, day cares, playgroups, and nurseries) and immunization centers in Zaria metropolis, Kaduna state, Nigeria. Data were collected between the months of November 2013 and May 2014. Zaria metropolis is a semi-urban region located within the Guinea Savannah belt of Nigeria, about 70 km North of Kaduna, the capital city of Kaduna state, North Western Nigeria.

Sampling method

Multistage sampling method was used to select the preschools and immunization centers while systematic sampling method was used to recruit the participants. Two local government areas (LGA) make up the Zaria metropolis: Zaria and Sabon gari LGAs. In stage I, two wards were selected from each LGA. In stage II, four immunization centers were selected from the wards by simple random sampling (one immunization center per ward). The list of schools for each ward was also obtained from the Primary School Management Board, and the schools were sampled proportioned to the number of schools per ward. A total of ten preschools were selected. Stage III involved the selection of children. In the schools, eligible pupils were selected by systematic sampling using the class register. The participants in the immunization centers were also selected by systematic sampling, repeating this during each visit until the sample size was achieved.

The minimal sample size for the study was determined using the formula:

n = z²pq/d²

Where n = the desired sample size; z = standard normal deviate corresponding to 95% confidence interval (CI) =1.96; p = prevalence of delay in visual skill (assumed to be 50% since no previous published study in Nigeria was found); q = 1 − p (the proportion of children without delay in visual skill); d = degree of accuracy desired (0.05).

For this study, z = 1.96, p = 0.50, q = 1 − 0.50 = 0.50, d = 0.05.

n = (1.96)² × 0.50 × 0.50/(0.05)² =384 children.

Allowance of 10% was made for nonresponse.

n = 38.4 + 384 = 422 children.

Selection criteria

Children aged 6–59 months found in the selected preschools and immunization centers and whose parents/guardians consented were included in this study. Children with previously diagnosed or obvious visual impairment were excluded from the study.

Ethical consideration

Approval of the Scientific and Health Research Ethics Committee of the Ahmadu Bello University Teaching Hospital Zaria and the Primary School Management Board were obtained. Written consent was obtained from the parents/caregivers of the participating children.

Data collection

The sociodemographic data were obtained from parents or guardians of the children using researcher administered prestructured questionnaire. Chronological age (CA) was ascertained using either record from school, birth certificate, or the immunization card. Households of six members and less were classified as small household size while those with more than six members were classified as large households. Father's monthly income was as reported by the father or the mother, where the information is known to her. The rate of exchange used was 199 naira= $1. The visual skill was assessed with the Schedule of Growing Skills II (SGS II) tool.[8]

Visual skill developmental assessment

The SGS II tool[8] was used to assess each child. The tool assesses the visual skill area along with 9 other skill areas (passive postural, active postural, locomotor, manipulative, hearing and language, speech and language, interactive social, self-care social, and cognitive skill).

The visual skill set contains two subsets: visual function and visual comprehension. The visual function assesses the child's ability to turn toward diffuse light, fixate, track objects through 90° and 180°, convergence on approaching object, and ability to finger point accurately at small objects. The visual comprehension tests for object permanence and scanning, discrimination of objects to identify objects by shape and outline, discrimination of details to identify actions, matching of objects by colors, and perception of patterns. The Snellen linear chart at 6 m was used to assess vision formally.

The developmental age (DA) in the visual skill area was obtained from the “Profile form” of the tool. The developmental quotient (DQ) in visual skill was obtained using the formula:

DQ (%) = DA/CA × 100

The DQ was then classified into:[9] Normal (DQ ≥85%) and delay (DQ ≤84%). Delay was recategorized into mild-moderate delay (DQ 71%–84%) and severe delay (DQ ≤70%). Children who required further visual evaluation were referred to the ophthalmologist.

Statistical analysis

Data were checked for completeness, coded, and analyzed using SPSS version 20.0.0. (Armonk, NY: IBM Corp) Descriptive analysis was used to calculate the frequencies and percentages of the respondents by sociodemographic characteristics. Binary logistic regression analysis was performed; the crude odds ratio was estimated to assess the association between each independent variable (e.g., household size, child's birth order, and mother's educational level,), and the dependent variable (developmental delay) and to select significant variables for multivariate logistic regression analysis. Variables which showed an association with dependent variable in the binary logistic regression analyses at alpha <0.05 were entered into multivariate logistic regression model. Adjusted odds ratios (AOR) with corresponding 95% CIs (95% CI) were used to analyze and interpret study results.


   Results Top


From a total of 422 children selected, 415 completed the assessment (98.3% response rate) and were included in the analysis.

Sociodemographic characteristics

[Table 1] show the age range was 6–59 months and mean age of 32.6 ± 15.9 months. The male to female ratio was 1.2:1. Two hundred and twenty-seven (54.7%) mothers had tertiary level of education, while 34 (8.2%) had no formal education. One hundred and thirty (31.3%) fathers had monthly income of 20,000 naira (approximately $100) or less. One hundred and five (25.3%) came from household size of more than six members.
Table 1: Sociodemographic characteristics of study population (n=415)

Click here to view


Mean developmental quotient (± standard deviation) of visual skill

[Table 2] shows that the mean DQ was highest among infants, with male infant mean DQ of 111 ± 12.7 and female infant mean DQ of 107 ± 11.8 compared to the other age groups. There was a significant gender difference in mean DQ among preschoolers aged 48–59 months. Overall mean DQ for visual skill was 100 ± 17.5%.
Table 2: Means±standard deviation of visual skill developmental quotient by age and sex

Click here to view


Prevalence of delay in visual skill

[Table 3] shows that overall prevalence of delay in visual skill is 17.1%, with 11.6% having mild-moderate delay, and 5.5% being severely delayed.
Table 3: Prevalence of delay in visual skill

Click here to view


Environmental risk factors associated with visual skill

[Table 4] shows that children of the first birth order were nearly two times more likely to have delay in visual skill area compared to other birth orders (AOR 1.83; 95% CI 1.05–3.30). The table also shows that children in large household size had more than twice the likelihood of delay in visual skill (AOR 2.34; 95% CI 1.32–2.14) compared to their counterparts from small households. Mother's educational level and the father's income also had significant association with delay in the visual domain.
Table 4: Environmental risk factors associated with developmental delay in visual skill

Click here to view



   Discussion Top


The high mean DQ observed in this study during infancy and a lower mean DQ by age 48–59 months corroborates with reports from other studies.[10],[11] This finding suggests that maturation of the visual skill is not uniform across the stages of development. As an infant explores and searches for information in the environment, his visual processing ability is stimulated. However, as search skill diminishes with age, the visual processing stimulation diminishes as the child grows older and by age six this stimulation would have stabilized.[12] Our study also showed significant gender difference in visual skill DQ by age 48–59 months in favor of boys. The previous studies have also shown sex difference in several facets of vision in favor of boys[13],[14] while some others reported difference in favor of girls.[15],[16] The finding in this study can be a reflection of the effect of gender stereotypic behavior of children within this age group having influence on their visual skill development. It has been shown that as children grow older, they regulate and enact behavior that is socially linked to gender.[17],[18] Boys compared to girls tend to be more engaged in activities that stimulate visual skill development such as mental rotation, map reading, targeting, embedded figures, sporting activities, video games, and play with large mobile toys such as trucks.[18],[19],[20],[21]

It was observed in this study that the prevalence rate of delayed visual skill was quite high. This prevalence was higher than prevalence rate of 10.5% and 4.4% obtained in other studies.[22],[23] The high prevalence rate obtained in our study may be because our assessment focused on different aspects of the child's visual system such as visual efficiency, visual information processing, and spatial skills. Previous studies[22],[23] focused on assessing single aspect of visual functional system either visual acuity or only visual perception. A high prevalence rate as observed in this study underpins the need for early assessment of every area of the visual system, other than visual acuity alone, especially as the preschool and early school years place relatively great demand on the child's visual skills.

Our study set out to determine environmental risk factors associated with delay in the visual skill development of children. We observed that children who were “ first born” and those who were members of large households had increased odds of delay in the visual skill area. This finding supports the fact that certain familial environmental factors influence visual skill development and probably child development as a whole.[11] Children of higher birth order, compared to first-born children, have the benefit of more experienced parents, exposure to stimulating toys, books, and other such materials and also benefit of added stimulation from older siblings.[24] With added siblings, however, large family size can sometimes have negative effect on child developmental outcomes, as observed in this study. However, it has also been shown that the irrespective of household size; within family influences have greater impact on child development.[25]

In this study, we found that delay in visual skill development was about two times more likely for children whose mothers had secondary school level of education and below (i.e., primary school and no formal education). This is in keeping with the previous studies[26],[27],[28] that have consistently shown the negative effect of low maternal education on visual skill development and child development as a whole. Highly educated mothers are more likely to invest in their children through stimulating materials such as toys, books, computers, special lessons, cognitive stimulating games[28] not only because they know the benefit of such exposure to the child's development but also because they have the financial resources to do so. This present study also showed that children whose fathers had low income had nearly twice the odds of delay in visual skill compared to children whose fathers had better income. A father's income reflects the family financial resources to a considerable extent, in addition to being an important resource for children in terms of provision of materials and activities needed for their development.[29] Just like low maternal education, thin financial resources limit the availability of materials that can stimulate visual skill development as preference for what is extremely essential is often made over what may be considered luxury or even waste.

Findings from this study provide information on visual skill development of under-fives, demonstrating that even apparently well children may exhibit delay in their visual system. The practical implication is to reinforce that the assessment of visual behavior during the first 5 years of life will allow us to verify how the child uses his vision to build his sensorimotor world as vision integrates other systems and senses. The knowledge of the environmental factors influencing this visual system during the early years justifies the need to assess their effect on the child's visual development at regular intervals.

There has been no study before this which had the primary aim of establishing visual skill developmental pattern of under-fives in Nigeria. Studies found were only for targeted ocular conditions. Meanwhile, studies such as this present one aims at early identification of visual developmental deviations which are not cosmetically obvious and are likely to be missed without assessment. The visual skill set in the SGS II tool is formulated with the consideration of DA and distinguishing developmental characteristics. It therefore cuts across geographic boundaries. Despite these strengths, this study is not without its limitations. First, the cross-sectional design of the study limits conclusions to be drawn about the causality. Second, a study across other developing countries would have given a wider perspective of the developmental pattern of the visual skill and the interplay with other environmental factors peculiar to the locality.


   Conclusion Top


Our findings show that delay in visual skill is quite common and that there is a significant gender difference in visual skill development for children aged between 48 and 59 months. Environmental factors such as first birth order, large family size, low father's income, and low maternal educational level were associated with delay in the visual skill area. Early identification of such delay and the environmental risk factors responsible will definitely have an impact on the improvement of visual skills, especially during the vulnerable first 5 years of life. We propose further research to compare the visual behavior with their school performance.

Financial support and sponsorship

Nil.

Conflicts of interest

There are no conflicts of interest.

 
   References Top

1.
Orlansky MD. Assessment of visually impaired infants and preschool children. In: Wachs TD, Sheehan R, editors. Assessment of Developmentally Disabled Children. New York: Springer Science and Business Media; 1998. p. 93-103.  Back to cited text no. 1
    
2.
Borsting E. Visual perception and reading. In: Garzia RP, editor. Vision and Reading. St. Louis, MO: Mosby-Year Book; 1996.  Back to cited text no. 2
    
3.
Elbaum B, Vaughn S. Self-concept and students with learning disabilities. In: Swanson HL, Graham S, Harris KR, editors. Handbook of Learning Disabilities. New York: Guilford Press; 2005.  Back to cited text no. 3
    
4.
Nowicki EA. A meta-analysis of the social competence of children with learning disabilities compared to classmates of low and average to high achievement. Learn Disabil Q 2003;26:171-88.  Back to cited text no. 4
    
5.
Sonksen PM, Dale N. Visual impairment in infancy: Impact on neurodevelopmental and neurobiological processes. Dev Med Child Neurol 2002;44:782-91.  Back to cited text no. 5
[PUBMED]    
6.
Smith M, Mikulecky L, Kibby MW, Dreher MJ, Dole JA. What will be the demands of literacy in the workplace in the next millennium? Read Res Q 2000;35:378-83.  Back to cited text no. 6
    
7.
de Koning HJ, Groenewoud JH, Lantau VK, Tjiam AM, Hoogeveen WC, de Faber JT, et al. Effectiveness of screening for amblyopia and other eye disorders in a prospective birth cohort study. J Med Screen 2013;20:66-72.  Back to cited text no. 7
[PUBMED]    
8.
Martin B, Sunandra L, Aukett A. Schedule of Growing Skills II. Berkshire, UK: GL Assessment Ltd.; 1996.  Back to cited text no. 8
    
9.
Robert BK, editor. Development and developmental disabilities. In: Manual of Clinical Problems in Pediatrics. UK: Lippincott Williams Wilkins; 2000. p. 29.  Back to cited text no. 9
    
10.
Goldeberg MC, Maurer D, Lewis TL. Developmental changes in attention: The effects of endogenous cueing and of distractors. Dev Sci 2001;4:209-19.  Back to cited text no. 10
    
11.
Scheiman MM, Rouse MW, Leslie S. Optometric management of learning-related vision problems. Clin Exp Optom 1995;78:119-20.  Back to cited text no. 11
    
12.
Dye MW, Bavelier D. Differential development of visual attention skills in school-age children. Vision Res 2010;50:452-9.  Back to cited text no. 12
[PUBMED]    
13.
Horwood AM, Riddell PM. Gender differences in early accommodation and vergence development. Ophthalmic Physiol Opt 2008;28:115-26.  Back to cited text no. 13
[PUBMED]    
14.
Dobkins KR, Bosworth RG, McCleery JP. Effects of gestational length, gender, postnatal age, and birth order on visual contrast sensitivity in infants. J Vis 2009 30;9:19.1-21.  Back to cited text no. 14
    
15.
Malcolm CA, McCulloch DL, Shepherd AJ. Pattern-reversal visual evoked potentials in infants: Gender differences during early visual maturation. Dev Med Child Neurol 2002;44:345-51.  Back to cited text no. 15
    
16.
Makrides M, Neumann MA, Gibson RA. Perinatal characteristics may influence the outcome of visual acuity. Lipids 2001;36:897-900.  Back to cited text no. 16
    
17.
Uzgiris IC, Kuper JC. The links between imitation and social referencing. In: Feinman S, editor. Social Referencing and the Social Construction of Reality in Infancy. New York: Plenum; 1992. p. 115-48.  Back to cited text no. 17
    
18.
Cherney ID, London K. Gender-linked differences in the toys, television shows, computer games and outdoor activities of 5-13 year old children. Sex Roles 2006;54:717-26.  Back to cited text no. 18
    
19.
Baron-Cohen S, Knickmeyer RC, Belmonte MK. Sex differences in the brain: Implications for explaining autism. Science 2005;310:819-23.  Back to cited text no. 19
    
20.
Subrahmanyam K, Greenfield PM. Effect of video game practice on spatial skills in girls and boys. J Appl Dev Psychol 1994;15:13-32.  Back to cited text no. 20
    
21.
Block JH. Differential premises arising from differential socialization of the sexes: Some conjectures. Child Dev 1983;54:1335-54.  Back to cited text no. 21
    
22.
Nielsen LS, Skov L, Jensen H. Visual dysfunctions and ocular disorders in children with developmental delay. I. prevalence, diagnoses and aetiology of visual impairment. Acta Ophthalmol Scand 2007;85:149-56.  Back to cited text no. 22
    
23.
Stich HL, Baune BT, Caniato RN, Mikolajczyk RT, Krämer A. Individual development of preschool children-prevalences and determinants of delays in Germany: A cross-sectional study in Southern Bavaria. BMC Pediatr 2012;12:188.  Back to cited text no. 23
    
24.
Heiland F. Does the birth order affect the cognitive development of a child? Appl Econ 2009;41:1799-818.  Back to cited text no. 24
    
25.
Becker GS, Lewis HG. On the interaction between the quantity and quality of children. J Polit Econ 1973;81:S279-88.  Back to cited text no. 25
    
26.
Tarczy-Hornoch K, Varma R, Cotter SA, McKean-Cowdin R, Lin JH, Borchert MS, et al. Risk factors for decreased visual acuity in preschool children: The multi-ethnic pediatric eye disease and Baltimore pediatric eye disease studies. Ophthalmology 2011;118:2262-73.  Back to cited text no. 26
    
27.
Wamani H, Tylleskär T, Šstrøm AN, Tumwine JK, Peterson S. Mothers' education but not fathers' education, household assets or land ownership is the best predictor of child health inequalities in rural Uganda. Int J Equity Health 2004;3:9.  Back to cited text no. 27
    
28.
Carneiro P, Meghir C, Parey M. Maternal education, home environments and the development of children and adolescents. J Eur Econ Assoc 2013;149:191-208.  Back to cited text no. 28
    
29.
Carlson MJ, Magnuson KA. Low-Income Men and Fathers' Influences on Children? Ann Am Acad Pol Soc Sci 2011;635:95-116.  Back to cited text no. 29
    



 
 
    Tables

  [Table 1], [Table 2], [Table 3], [Table 4]



 

Top
  
 
  Search
 
    Similar in PUBMED
   Search Pubmed for
   Search in Google Scholar for
 Related articles
    Access Statistics
    Email Alert *
    Add to My List *
* Registration required (free)  

 
  In this article
    Abstract
   Introduction
   Subjects and Methods
   Results
   Discussion
   Conclusion
    References
    Article Tables

 Article Access Statistics
    Viewed1820    
    Printed45    
    Emailed0    
    PDF Downloaded184    
    Comments [Add]    

Recommend this journal